What does hippocampus do




















The hippocampus is found in the temporal lobe below the cerebral cortex. Although we often refer to it in the singular, there are actually two hippocampi—one in each cerebral hemisphere. The term hippocampus comes from the Greek word for seahorse, because when it is removed from the brain, the hippocampus vaguely resembles a seahorse see picture below.

The hippocampus is a brain structure thought to play a critical role in memory. Although suspected to be involved with memory for some time, the importance of the hippocampus in this respect was solidified in the twentieth century by the case of a patient named Henry Molaison. Molaison, who was known simply as H. In that surgery, much of his hippocampi were either removed or damaged.

The surgery was successful in controlling Molaison's seizures, but afterwards he suffered from severe anterograde amnesia , meaning that his ability to form new memories was impaired. In fact, Molaison was completely unable to form new explicit memories memories that involve conscious recollection of facts or experiences.

Molaison retained some memories from before the surgery—although memories that were closer in time to his surgery were less stable—and his procedural memory memory for non-conscious procedures like using a fork or riding a bike was still functional.

But due to his implicit memory deficits, Molaison was forced to live entirely in the present; each day brought with it no recollection of the day before. Another patient named Clive Wearing experienced hippocampal damage after suffering from encephalitis , and is living with a deficit very similar to Molaison's. Neuron 37, — Hassabis, D.

Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Heckers, S. Impaired recruitment of the hippocampus during conscious recollection in schizophrenia. Heim, C. The link between childhood trauma and depression: insights from HPA axis studies in humans.

Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, — Henke, K. A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Hynes, C. Differential role of the orbital frontal lobe in emotional versus cognitive perspective-taking.

Jacobson, L. The role of the hippocampus in feedback regulation of the hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Janowsky, J.

Cognitive impairment following frontal lobe damage and its relevance to human amnesia. Johnson, M. Jurado, M. The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Knowlton, B. Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. Konkel, A. Hippocampal amnesia impairs all manner of relational memory. Kowatari, Y. Neural networks involved in artistic creativity. Brain Mapp. Kumaran, D.

The human hippocampus: cognitive maps or relational memory? Kurczek, J. Hippocampal contributions to language: evidence of referential processing deficits in amnesia. Kwan, D. Deficits in past remembering extend to future imagining in a case of developmental amnesia.

LaBar, K. Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Lavenex, P. Hippocampal-neocortical interaction: a hierarchy of associativity. Hippocampus 10, — LeDoux, J. The emotional brain, fear and the amygdala. Lemke, J. Across the scales of time: artifacts, activities and meanings in ecosocial systems.

Mind Cult. Leng, N. Double dissociation of frontal dysfunction in organic amnesia. Lezak, M. Newer contributions to the neuropsychological assessment of executive functions. Head Trauma Rehabil. Loftus, E. Eyewitness Testimony: Civil and Criminal 4th Edn. The formation of false memories. MacQueen, G. Course of illness, hippocampal function and hippocampal volume in major depression. Maguire, E. Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network.

Recalling routes around London: activation of the right hippocampus in taxi drivers. Navigation around London by a taxi driver with bilateral hippocampal lesions. Brain Pt. The hippocampus: a manifesto for change. Mah, L. Deficits in social knowledge following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

Neuropsychiatry Clin. McGaugh, J. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Mervaala, E. Quantitative MRI of the hippocampus and amygdala in severe depression. Moll, J. The moral affiliations of disgust: a functional MRI study. Moscovitch, M. Neisser, U. Memory Observed: Remembering in Natural Contexts. San Francisco: Freeman. Nelson, M. Hippocampal volume reduction in schizophrenia as assessed by magnetic resonance imaging: a meta-analytic study.

Psychiatry 55, — Norman, K. Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: a complementary-learning-systems approach. Norris, C. The interaction of social and emotional processes in the brain. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Olson, I. Visual working memory is impaired when the medial temporal lobe is damaged. Olsen, R. The hippocampus supports multiple cognitive processes through relational binding and comparison. Working memory for conjunctions relies on the medial temporal lobe.

The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cortex 10, — Phelps, E. Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. Pikkarainen, M. Projections from the lateral, basal and accessory basal nuclei of the amygdala to the hippocampal formation in rat.

Posener, J. High-dimensional mapping of the hippocampus in depression. Race, E. Losing sight of the future: impaired semantic prospection following medial temporal lobe lesions. Ranganath, C. A unified framework for the functional organization of the medial temporal lobes and the phenomenology of episodic memory. Hippocampus 20, — Binding items and contexts: the cognitive neuroscience of episodic memory.

Medial temporal lobe activity associated with active maintenance of novel information. Neuron 31, — Richardson, M. Encoding of emotional memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus and their interactions. Rilling, J. A neural basis for social cooperation. Rogers, R. Distinct portions of anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex are activated by reward processing in separable phases of decision-making cognition.

Rosene, D. Hippocampal efferents reach widespread areas of cerebral cortex and amygdala in the rhesus monkey. Rubin, R. How do I remember that I know you know that I know? Runco, M. Ryan, J. Amnesia is a deficit in relational memory.

Sabbagh, M. Understanding orbitofrontal contributions to theory-of-mind reasoning: implications for autism. Brain Cogn. Schacter, D. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future. On the nature of medial temporal lobe contributions to the constructive simulation of future events. Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain.

Schumann, C. The amygdala is enlarged in children but not adolescents with autism; the hippocampus is enlarged at all ages. Scoville, W. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions.

Psychiatry 20, 11— Shin, L. Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex and hippocampal function in PTSD. Shoqeirat, M. Performance on tests sensitive to frontal lobe lesions by patients with organic amnesia: Leng and Parkin revisited. Shuren, J. The neurology of reasoning. Simons, J. Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory. Sinz, H. Neuropsychologia 46, — Smith, E. Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes.

Spiers, H. Thoughts, behaviour and brain dynamics during navigation in the real world. Neuroimage 31, — Spreng, R. Examining the role of memory in social cognition.

Squire, L. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys and humans. Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: a new perspective. The medial temporal lobe memory system.

Staresina, B. Mind the gap: binding experiences across space and time in the human hippocampus. Neuron 63, — Stiller, J. Perspective-taking and memory capacity predict social network size. Networks 29, 93— Stone, V. Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia 41, — Stuss, D.

Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Adult clinical neuropsychology: lessons from studies of the frontal lobes.

Sutherland, R. Contributions of cingulate cortex to two forms of spatial learning and memory. Szpunar, K. Episodic future thought and its relation to remembering: evidence from ratings of subjective experience. Tanji, J. Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in executive behavioral control. Tannen, D. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Todorov, A. Robust learning of affective trait associations with faces when the hippocampus is damaged, but not when the amygdala and temporal pole are damaged.

Tranel, D. The covert learning of affective valence does not require structures in hippocampal system or amygdala. Voss, J. Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitional exploratory behavior enhances learning. Spontaneous revisitation during visual exploration as a link among strategic behavior, learning and the hippocampus. U S A , E—E Vuilleumier, P. Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study.

Neuron 30, — Wang, J. Covert rapid action-memory simulation CRAMS : a hypothesis of hippocampal-prefrontal interactions for adaptive behavior. Warren, D. Long-term neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and life outcome in hippocampal amnesia. Observing degradation of visual representations over short intervals when medial temporal lobe is damaged.

Watson, P. Spatial reconstruction by patients with hippocampal damage is dominated by relational memory errors. Williams, L. Eye-movement behavior reveals relational memory impairment in schizophrenia. Psychiatry 68, — Wimmer, G. Preference by association: how memory mechanisms in the hippocampus bias decisions. Wood, J. Human prefrontal cortex: processing and representational perspectives. Wyass, J. Connections between the retrosplenial cortex and the hippocampal formation in the rat: a review.

Hippocampus 2, 1— Yee, L. The hippocampus uses information just encountered to guide efficient ongoing behavior. Hippocampus 24, — Yonelinas, A. The hippocampus supports high-resolution binding in the service of perception, working memory and long-term memory. Zeithamova, D. The hippocampus and inferential reasoning: building memories to navigate future decisions. Keywords: hippocampus, flexible cognition, social behavior, relational memory, amnesia. The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author s or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice.

No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. Mathews Ave. The role of the hippocampus in flexible cognition and social behavior Rachael D.

Watson 1 , Melissa C. Duff 2,3 and Neal J. Cohen 1. The Network of Brain Structures Supporting Flexible Cognition and the Hippocampus Flexible cognition is often discussed in the context of executive function, supporting the ability to switch between competing goals, as well as contributing to high-level human behavior, such as planning, organizing, and decision-making e. The Hippocampus Supports Flexible Cognition Through the Encoding and Flexible Expression of Relational Memory Representations Early neuropsychological studies in patients with hippocampal amnesia provided crucial insight into the organization of human memory and its instantiation in the brain, such that damage to the hippocampus and related medial temporal lobe MTL structures resulted in a profound but circumscribed amnesia e.

The Hippocampus and Flexible Memory Representations are Critical in Many Cognitive Abilities and Complex Social Behaviors We suggest that the flexibility afforded by hippocampal representations permits various pieces of information to be called upon promiscuously to support diverse and complex cognitive and social abilities. Research suggests that exercise may help protect the hippocampus from the detrimental effects of aging.

Some research suggests that stress associated with PTSD may also lead to damage to the hippocampus. There are a few different factors that can affect the function of the hippocampus:.

Age can also have a major impact on the functioning of the hippocampus. Those who experience such a loss may show significant declines in memory performance. The hippocampus may also play a role in contributing to the development of addictions. Because drugs and alcohol affect the brain's reward systems, the hippocampus creates memories of these satisfying experiences.

It also may help form memories of environmental cues associated with substance use that can contribute to intense cravings when these cues are encountered again. The term hippocampus is derived from the Greek word hippokampus hippo meaning "horse" and kampos meaning "sea monster" because the structure resembles the shape of a sea horse. The structure was first described by the anatomist Julius Caesar Aranzi. Because the hippocampus has been known of and observed for centuries, it is one of the most studied areas of the brain.

Learn the best ways to manage stress and negativity in your life. The Influences of Emotion on Learning and Memory. Front Psychol. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers.

Remembering Preservation in Hippocampal Amnesia. Annu Rev Psychol. Left-right dissociation of hippocampal memory processes in mice. Can physical exercise in old age improve memory and hippocampal function?

Stress effects on the hippocampus: A critical review. Learn Mem. Biol Psychiatry. Aging of the cerebral cortex differs between humans and chimpanzees. Anand KS, Dhikav V. Hippocampus in health and disease: An overview.

Ann Indian Acad Neurol. Harvard Health Publishing. What is addiction? Myers DG. Exploring Psychology.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000